Wharton Digital Press Logo

This website does not support versions of Internet Explorer below version 9. Please try updating your version of Internet Explorer or using another browser. Thank You.

Robert Meyer and Howard Kunreuther  |  May 22, 2017

6 Reasons We Underprepare for Disasters—And What Can Be Done About It

Share about this on Facebook Tweet about this on Twitter Share on LinkedIn

In the new book The Ostrich Paradox: Why We Underprepare for Disasters, Robert Meyer and Howard Kunreuther explore the reasons that individuals, communities, and institutions often underinvest in protection against low-probability, high-consequence events—and what can be done about it. In this excerpt from The Ostrich Paradox, called “a must-read” by Daniel Kahneman, winner of the Nobel Prize in Economics and author of Thinking, Fast and Slow, Meyer and Kunreuther look at why history often repeats itself.

When dawn broke on the morning of September 8, 1900, the people of Galveston had no inkling of the disaster that was about to befall them. The thickening clouds and rising surf hinted that a storm was on the way, but few were worried.

The local weather bureau office, for its part, gave no reason to think otherwise; no urgent warnings were issued, no calls were made to evacuate. But by late afternoon it became clear that this was no ordinary storm. Hurricane-force winds of more than 100 miles per hour were soon raking the city, driving a massive storm surge that devoured almost everything in its path. Many tried to flee, but it was too late. By the next day, more than 8,000 people were dead, the greatest loss of life from a natural disaster in US history.

“Warnings were issued, but many in low-lying coastal communities ignored them—even when told that failing to heed the warnings meant they faced certain death.”

Fast-forward to September 2008, when Hurricane Ike threatened the same part of the Texas coast, but this time being greeted by a well-informed populace. Ike had been under constant surveillance by satellites, aircraft reconnaissance, and land-based radar for more than a week, with the news media blasting a nonstop cacophony of reports and warnings, urging those in coastal areas to leave. The city of Galveston was also well prepared: A 17-foot-high seawall that had been constructed after the 1900 storm stood ready to protect the city, and government-flood insurance policies were available to residents who were at risk of property loss. Unlike in 1900, Texas residents really should have had little reason to fear. On their side was a century of advances in meteorology, engineering, and economics designed to ensure that Ike would, indeed, pass as a forgettable summer storm.

But for some reason it didn’t quite work out that way. Warnings were issued, but many in low-lying coastal communities ignored them—even when told that failing to heed the warnings meant they faced certain death. Galveston’s aging seawall was breached in multiple places, damaging up to 80% of homes and businesses in the city. The resort communities to the north on the Bolivar Peninsula, which never saw the need for a seawall, fared even worse, witnessing almost complete destruction. And among the thousands of homeowners who suffered flood losses, only 39% had seen fit to purchase flood insurance. In the end, Ike caused more than $14 billion in property damage and 100 deaths, almost all of it needless.

“While our ability to foresee and protect against natural catastrophes has increased dramatically over the course of the past century, it has done little to reduce material losses from such events.”

Why Are We Underprepared for Disasters?

The gap between protective technology and protective action illustrated by the losses in Hurricane Ike is, of course, hardly limited to Galveston or to hurricanes. While our ability to foresee and protect against natural catastrophes has increased dramatically over the course of the past century, it has done little to reduce material losses from such events.

Rather than seeing decreases in damage and fatalities due to the aid of science, we’ve instead seen the worldwide economic cost and impact on people’s lives from hazards increase exponentially through the early twenty-first century, with five of the 10 costliest natural disasters in history with respect to property damage occurring since 2005. While scientific and technological advances have allowed deaths to decrease on average, horrific calamities still occur, as in the case of the 230,000 people estimated to have lost their lives in the 2004 Indian Ocean earthquake and tsunami, the 87,000 who died in the 2008 Sichuan earthquake in China, the 160,000 who lost their lives in Haiti from an earthquake in 2010, and the 8,000 fatalities that occurred in the 2015 Nepalese earthquake. Even in the United States, Hurricane Katrina in 2005 caused more than 1,800 fatalities, making it the third most deadly such storm in US history.

In our book The Ostrich Paradox, we explore six reasons that individuals, communities, and institutions often underinvest in protection against low-probability, high-consequence events. They are:

  1. Myopia: a tendency to focus on overly short future time horizons when appraising immediate costs and the potential benefits of protective investments;
  2. Amnesia: a tendency to forget too quickly the lessons of past disasters;
  3. Optimism: a tendency to underestimate the likelihood that losses will occur from future hazards;
  4. Inertia: a tendency to maintain the status quo or adopt a default option when there is uncertainty about the potential benefits of investing in alternative protective measures;
  5. Simplification: a tendency to selectively attend to only a subset of the relevant factors to consider when making choices involving risk; and
  6. Herding: a tendency to base choices on the observed actions of others.

We need to recognize that when making decisions, our biases are part of our cognitive DNA. While we may not be able to alter our cognitive wiring, we may be able to improve preparedness by recognizing these specific biases and designing strategies that anticipate them.

To learn more about the book, visit wsp.wharton.upenn.edu/book/ostrich-paradox/.